Lucy O'Brien
- Published in print:
- 2007
- Published Online:
- September 2007
- ISBN:
- 9780199261482
- eISBN:
- 9780191718632
- Item type:
- chapter
- Publisher:
- Oxford University Press
- DOI:
- 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199261482.003.0010
- Subject:
- Philosophy, Philosophy of Mind
This chapter explores the claim that bodily awareness is a perceptual faculty or, more realistically, set of faculties. It shows that there are a number of ways to construe the widely accepted claim ...
More
This chapter explores the claim that bodily awareness is a perceptual faculty or, more realistically, set of faculties. It shows that there are a number of ways to construe the widely accepted claim that bodily awareness is a perceptual faculty. Bodily awareness can be construed as a faculty which enables us to perceive properties of our bodies — its shape, location, movement, as well as phenomenal pain properties, tickle properties, and the like. Unless one is a sceptic about secondary properties, or has a specific reason for thinking that there could not be phenomenal perceptible properties of our bodies, there is no impediment to doing so.Less
This chapter explores the claim that bodily awareness is a perceptual faculty or, more realistically, set of faculties. It shows that there are a number of ways to construe the widely accepted claim that bodily awareness is a perceptual faculty. Bodily awareness can be construed as a faculty which enables us to perceive properties of our bodies — its shape, location, movement, as well as phenomenal pain properties, tickle properties, and the like. Unless one is a sceptic about secondary properties, or has a specific reason for thinking that there could not be phenomenal perceptible properties of our bodies, there is no impediment to doing so.
Tony Russell
- Published in print:
- 2021
- Published Online:
- March 2021
- ISBN:
- 9780190091187
- eISBN:
- 9780190091217
- Item type:
- chapter
- Publisher:
- Oxford University Press
- DOI:
- 10.1093/oso/9780190091187.003.0021
- Subject:
- Music, History, American, Popular
This chapter discusses New Arkansas Travelers, “Handy Man”, “I Tickled ’Em”, and music hall song
This chapter discusses New Arkansas Travelers, “Handy Man”, “I Tickled ’Em”, and music hall song
Vic Hobson
- Published in print:
- 2014
- Published Online:
- September 2014
- ISBN:
- 9781617039911
- eISBN:
- 9781626740259
- Item type:
- chapter
- Publisher:
- University Press of Mississippi
- DOI:
- 10.14325/mississippi/9781617039911.003.0002
- Subject:
- Music, History, American
Jazzmen promoted Buddy Bolden as the legendary first man of jazz. Willy Cornish played with Bolden. Cornish was in a photograph of Bolden’s original band. Bunk Johnson too claimed to have played with ...
More
Jazzmen promoted Buddy Bolden as the legendary first man of jazz. Willy Cornish played with Bolden. Cornish was in a photograph of Bolden’s original band. Bunk Johnson too claimed to have played with Bolden but was not in the photograph. Increasingly his claims did not seem credible. This chapter argues that the photograph was “tin type” photograph. That Bolden was not a barber; he did not write for the Cricket (although a friend Otis Watts did), that it was William Spillis who died on the Labor Day March in 1906, and that it is likely that Bolden made a cylinder recording because this information came from Willy Cornish. This chapter considers Bolden’s theme tune “Buddy Bolden’s Blues” and its relationship to ragtime tunes, “The Cake Walk in the Sky” (1899) and “The St. Louis Tickle” (1904).Less
Jazzmen promoted Buddy Bolden as the legendary first man of jazz. Willy Cornish played with Bolden. Cornish was in a photograph of Bolden’s original band. Bunk Johnson too claimed to have played with Bolden but was not in the photograph. Increasingly his claims did not seem credible. This chapter argues that the photograph was “tin type” photograph. That Bolden was not a barber; he did not write for the Cricket (although a friend Otis Watts did), that it was William Spillis who died on the Labor Day March in 1906, and that it is likely that Bolden made a cylinder recording because this information came from Willy Cornish. This chapter considers Bolden’s theme tune “Buddy Bolden’s Blues” and its relationship to ragtime tunes, “The Cake Walk in the Sky” (1899) and “The St. Louis Tickle” (1904).
Diana P. Szameitat, Dirk Wildgruber, and Kai Alter
- Published in print:
- 2013
- Published Online:
- May 2013
- ISBN:
- 9780199583560
- eISBN:
- 9780191747489
- Item type:
- chapter
- Publisher:
- Oxford University Press
- DOI:
- 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199583560.003.0012
- Subject:
- Psychology, Cognitive Psychology, Evolutionary Psychology
Human laughter is a complex behavior expressed not only in the context of play as it is in animals, but also in various emotional states. This suggests that throughout evolution human laughter has ...
More
Human laughter is a complex behavior expressed not only in the context of play as it is in animals, but also in various emotional states. This suggests that throughout evolution human laughter has adopted more complex communicative functions to reflect the increased complexity of human social interaction. This chapter outlines recent findings on perception, acoustic cues, and neural correlates of laughter sounds. Laughter sounds can be classified according to the emotional categories and according to different emotional dimensions, such as arousal, valence and dominance. Moreover, the acoustical correlates of laughter expressing different emotions can be discriminated on the basis of a small parameter set. Finally, the evolutionary diversification of ecological functions of laughter is associated with distinct cerebral responses underlying laughter perception. These findings indicate that human laughter is a multifaceted behavior serving distinct social functions.Less
Human laughter is a complex behavior expressed not only in the context of play as it is in animals, but also in various emotional states. This suggests that throughout evolution human laughter has adopted more complex communicative functions to reflect the increased complexity of human social interaction. This chapter outlines recent findings on perception, acoustic cues, and neural correlates of laughter sounds. Laughter sounds can be classified according to the emotional categories and according to different emotional dimensions, such as arousal, valence and dominance. Moreover, the acoustical correlates of laughter expressing different emotions can be discriminated on the basis of a small parameter set. Finally, the evolutionary diversification of ecological functions of laughter is associated with distinct cerebral responses underlying laughter perception. These findings indicate that human laughter is a multifaceted behavior serving distinct social functions.
Laura Frost
- Published in print:
- 2013
- Published Online:
- November 2015
- ISBN:
- 9780231152723
- eISBN:
- 9780231526463
- Item type:
- chapter
- Publisher:
- Columbia University Press
- DOI:
- 10.7312/columbia/9780231152723.003.0003
- Subject:
- Literature, 20th-century Literature and Modernism
This chapter examines how and whether Gertrude Stein gives pleasure. Focusing on Stein's experimental writing from 1914 to her lectures of the mid-1930s, it suggests a new model for approaching her ...
More
This chapter examines how and whether Gertrude Stein gives pleasure. Focusing on Stein's experimental writing from 1914 to her lectures of the mid-1930s, it suggests a new model for approaching her work: tickling. It considers tickling as an enduring social and scientific mystery and how its patterns illuminate Stein's methods and her reading effects. It also explores the ways that the gesture of tickling describes both the delights and difficulties of Stein's texts and also accounts for the wide discrepancies in readers' responses to her writing. Tickling, whose mysterious idiosyncrasies have intrigued theorists from Plato to Adam Phillips, characterizes Stein's infantile and erotic impulses, her abstraction and sensuality, and the sliding scale of pleasure to irritation that her work arouses. Stein's texts can be a pleasure machine or a torture machine or both, depending on the reader.Less
This chapter examines how and whether Gertrude Stein gives pleasure. Focusing on Stein's experimental writing from 1914 to her lectures of the mid-1930s, it suggests a new model for approaching her work: tickling. It considers tickling as an enduring social and scientific mystery and how its patterns illuminate Stein's methods and her reading effects. It also explores the ways that the gesture of tickling describes both the delights and difficulties of Stein's texts and also accounts for the wide discrepancies in readers' responses to her writing. Tickling, whose mysterious idiosyncrasies have intrigued theorists from Plato to Adam Phillips, characterizes Stein's infantile and erotic impulses, her abstraction and sensuality, and the sliding scale of pleasure to irritation that her work arouses. Stein's texts can be a pleasure machine or a torture machine or both, depending on the reader.
John Montgomery and David Bodznick
- Published in print:
- 2016
- Published Online:
- January 2017
- ISBN:
- 9780198758860
- eISBN:
- 9780191834752
- Item type:
- chapter
- Publisher:
- Oxford University Press
- DOI:
- 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198758860.003.0002
- Subject:
- Neuroscience, Development, Molecular and Cellular Systems
The idea of the cerebellar self provides a framework from which to explore cerebellar function, starting with the insight this provides for human psychotic states that fail to distinguish ‘self’ from ...
More
The idea of the cerebellar self provides a framework from which to explore cerebellar function, starting with the insight this provides for human psychotic states that fail to distinguish ‘self’ from ‘other’. One key task of cerebellar adaptive filters is to provide predictions of the consequences of our actions, called forward models, such as the sensory consequences of movements we make. For example, it is well known that an external tickle is more ticklish than self-tickle, an effect that can be attributed to the cerebellar attenuation of predictable input. Interestingly, people with some forms of psychoses do not experience the same difference, clearly implicating an underlying cerebellar dysfunction. A role for the cerebellar sense of self also extends to that compelling, but fragile, experience of controlling actions in the outside world. This is called our sense of agency, and it too depends on a match between expectations and eventualities.Less
The idea of the cerebellar self provides a framework from which to explore cerebellar function, starting with the insight this provides for human psychotic states that fail to distinguish ‘self’ from ‘other’. One key task of cerebellar adaptive filters is to provide predictions of the consequences of our actions, called forward models, such as the sensory consequences of movements we make. For example, it is well known that an external tickle is more ticklish than self-tickle, an effect that can be attributed to the cerebellar attenuation of predictable input. Interestingly, people with some forms of psychoses do not experience the same difference, clearly implicating an underlying cerebellar dysfunction. A role for the cerebellar sense of self also extends to that compelling, but fragile, experience of controlling actions in the outside world. This is called our sense of agency, and it too depends on a match between expectations and eventualities.
Joe Moshenska
- Published in print:
- 2014
- Published Online:
- November 2014
- ISBN:
- 9780198712947
- eISBN:
- 9780191781377
- Item type:
- book
- Publisher:
- Oxford University Press
- DOI:
- 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198712947.001.0001
- Subject:
- Literature, 16th-century and Renaissance Literature, Criticism/Theory
Feeling Pleasures identifies the sixteenth and seventeenth centuries in England as a period in which the sense of touch was subjected to unprecedented scrutiny and debate, through which a variety of ...
More
Feeling Pleasures identifies the sixteenth and seventeenth centuries in England as a period in which the sense of touch was subjected to unprecedented scrutiny and debate, through which a variety of writers and artists explored the most fundamental of questions concerning the nature and worth of the human body and its experiences, the place of the human in the material world and the relationship between the human and the divine. The opening chapters consider the debates surrounding pious touch occasioned by the Reformation, ranging from polemics aimed at the handling of relics and the Eucharist to the sermons of Lancelot Andrewes, which place intense hermeneutic pressure on the verb ‘to touch’ in several languages. Subsequent chapters explore the fluctuating availablility of the Greco-Roman gods to touch, and the convergence of these debates in the writings of Montaigne, and the poetry of Shakespeare and Spenser. The ways in which paintings and sculptures seemed both to invite and rebuff admiring touch are then considered, and the philosophical problems presented by the experience of tickling from antiquity to the writings of Descartes and Galileo. The uncertain place of touch in Paradise Lost is explored, where Milton makes touch a possible foundation for human experience while also exploring its devastating loss. Finally, the new understanding of touch suggested by dawning awareness of Chinese pulse-taking techniques is considered, which challenged established medical practice but also impacted on debates surrounding poetic rhythm and metaphor.Less
Feeling Pleasures identifies the sixteenth and seventeenth centuries in England as a period in which the sense of touch was subjected to unprecedented scrutiny and debate, through which a variety of writers and artists explored the most fundamental of questions concerning the nature and worth of the human body and its experiences, the place of the human in the material world and the relationship between the human and the divine. The opening chapters consider the debates surrounding pious touch occasioned by the Reformation, ranging from polemics aimed at the handling of relics and the Eucharist to the sermons of Lancelot Andrewes, which place intense hermeneutic pressure on the verb ‘to touch’ in several languages. Subsequent chapters explore the fluctuating availablility of the Greco-Roman gods to touch, and the convergence of these debates in the writings of Montaigne, and the poetry of Shakespeare and Spenser. The ways in which paintings and sculptures seemed both to invite and rebuff admiring touch are then considered, and the philosophical problems presented by the experience of tickling from antiquity to the writings of Descartes and Galileo. The uncertain place of touch in Paradise Lost is explored, where Milton makes touch a possible foundation for human experience while also exploring its devastating loss. Finally, the new understanding of touch suggested by dawning awareness of Chinese pulse-taking techniques is considered, which challenged established medical practice but also impacted on debates surrounding poetic rhythm and metaphor.
Joe Moshenska
- Published in print:
- 2014
- Published Online:
- November 2014
- ISBN:
- 9780198712947
- eISBN:
- 9780191781377
- Item type:
- chapter
- Publisher:
- Oxford University Press
- DOI:
- 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198712947.003.0007
- Subject:
- Literature, 16th-century and Renaissance Literature, Criticism/Theory
This chapter begins with the discussion of the problems presented by the experience of tickling in the pseudo-Aristotelian Problemata, including the mysterious facts that one cannot tickle oneself, ...
More
This chapter begins with the discussion of the problems presented by the experience of tickling in the pseudo-Aristotelian Problemata, including the mysterious facts that one cannot tickle oneself, and that only certain parts of the body are ticklish. Later discussions of tickling in the Renaissance are considered, including the writings of Erasmus, Richard Mulcaster, and Francis Bacon. The chapter considers Galileo and Descartes, both of whom explored experiences of tickling as part of their attempts to produce a transformed understanding of the basis of sensation. Descartes explores a moment of tickling on the threshold of conscious experience in order to displace the traditional reliability of touch, while Galileo observes that a statue cannot be tickled in order to argue that ticklishness is not a real quality that inheres in certain objects, such as feathers, but rather in the ticklish parts of the body.Less
This chapter begins with the discussion of the problems presented by the experience of tickling in the pseudo-Aristotelian Problemata, including the mysterious facts that one cannot tickle oneself, and that only certain parts of the body are ticklish. Later discussions of tickling in the Renaissance are considered, including the writings of Erasmus, Richard Mulcaster, and Francis Bacon. The chapter considers Galileo and Descartes, both of whom explored experiences of tickling as part of their attempts to produce a transformed understanding of the basis of sensation. Descartes explores a moment of tickling on the threshold of conscious experience in order to displace the traditional reliability of touch, while Galileo observes that a statue cannot be tickled in order to argue that ticklishness is not a real quality that inheres in certain objects, such as feathers, but rather in the ticklish parts of the body.
Bruno and
- Published in print:
- 2018
- Published Online:
- September 2018
- ISBN:
- 9780198725022
- eISBN:
- 9780191860041
- Item type:
- chapter
- Publisher:
- Oxford University Press
- DOI:
- 10.1093/oso/9780198725022.003.0003
- Subject:
- Psychology, Cognitive Psychology, Cognitive Neuroscience
Our bodies are not static, and multisensory signals are constantly being processed to produce motor behaviours. This chapter will discuss how multisensory interactions shape three kinds of such ...
More
Our bodies are not static, and multisensory signals are constantly being processed to produce motor behaviours. This chapter will discuss how multisensory interactions shape three kinds of such behaviours: reaching and grasping objects with the hand, walking, and maintaining one’s posture. Motor control is inherently multisensory, as it involves combining anticipatory sensory signals from vision and proprioception, as well as, in some cases, other sensory channels, to prepare movements before they are actually initiated, and then combining online multisensory feedback to control movements while they are being executed. In addition, multisensory motor processes turn out to be important in understanding how we perceive agency, the awareness that our own minds are the agents that will allow our actions to take place, how we adapt to novel sensory environments, how we understand actions performed by others exploiting ‘mirror’ sensorimotor brain systems, and perhaps even why we can’t tickle ourselves.Less
Our bodies are not static, and multisensory signals are constantly being processed to produce motor behaviours. This chapter will discuss how multisensory interactions shape three kinds of such behaviours: reaching and grasping objects with the hand, walking, and maintaining one’s posture. Motor control is inherently multisensory, as it involves combining anticipatory sensory signals from vision and proprioception, as well as, in some cases, other sensory channels, to prepare movements before they are actually initiated, and then combining online multisensory feedback to control movements while they are being executed. In addition, multisensory motor processes turn out to be important in understanding how we perceive agency, the awareness that our own minds are the agents that will allow our actions to take place, how we adapt to novel sensory environments, how we understand actions performed by others exploiting ‘mirror’ sensorimotor brain systems, and perhaps even why we can’t tickle ourselves.