P. J. Barber
- Published in print:
- 2013
- Published Online:
- April 2014
- ISBN:
- 9780199680504
- eISBN:
- 9780191760525
- Item type:
- chapter
- Publisher:
- Oxford University Press
- DOI:
- 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199680504.003.0004
- Subject:
- Classical Studies, Literary Studies: Classical, Early, and Medieval
Chapter 4 evaluates the evidence for Sievers’ Law in the distribution of the Greek primary comparative adjective suffixes *-yos-, *-yon-, *-iyos-, *-iyon-, *-yos-, and *-yon-. This chapter is ...
More
Chapter 4 evaluates the evidence for Sievers’ Law in the distribution of the Greek primary comparative adjective suffixes *-yos-, *-yon-, *-iyos-, *-iyon-, *-yos-, and *-yon-. This chapter is situated in Part II (Greek Nominal Categories). In order to understand the distribution of semivowels, it is first necessary to explain the s-stem and n-stem variant suffix forms and also the long *and short ∗ivariants. The impact of ablaut alternations is discussed. The evidence of each attested primary comparative adjective in Greek is discussed and set in the context of the history of the formation. Finally, the overall pattern of attestation is evaluated.Less
Chapter 4 evaluates the evidence for Sievers’ Law in the distribution of the Greek primary comparative adjective suffixes *-yos-, *-yon-, *-iyos-, *-iyon-, *-yos-, and *-yon-. This chapter is situated in Part II (Greek Nominal Categories). In order to understand the distribution of semivowels, it is first necessary to explain the s-stem and n-stem variant suffix forms and also the long *and short ∗ivariants. The impact of ablaut alternations is discussed. The evidence of each attested primary comparative adjective in Greek is discussed and set in the context of the history of the formation. Finally, the overall pattern of attestation is evaluated.
Matthew E. Adams
- Published in print:
- 2017
- Published Online:
- January 2017
- ISBN:
- 9780190210304
- eISBN:
- 9780190210328
- Item type:
- chapter
- Publisher:
- Oxford University Press
- DOI:
- 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780190210304.003.0008
- Subject:
- Linguistics, Phonetics / Phonology
This chapter argues that frequency, prosody, and morphology interact in principled ways to yield quantitative asymmetries in the morphosyntactic realization of the English comparative. The typical ...
More
This chapter argues that frequency, prosody, and morphology interact in principled ways to yield quantitative asymmetries in the morphosyntactic realization of the English comparative. The typical generalization is that length of the adjective determines whether or not -er is augmented by arguing that usage frequency creates distinct lexical entries for frequent and infrequent adjectives. In accordance with independent facts of differential susceptibility to lenition and phonetic reduction, the chapter argues that the relevant locus of difference lies in prosodic detail. Broadly, the comparative suffix -er is more amenable to attachment to prosodically incomplete adjectives, given its status as a cohering suffix. Under the theoretical principle of comparative grammaticality, whereby a competing way of expressing semantic comparison is available, the degree to which -er attaches to adjectives varies as a function of the prosodic completeness of the adjective, its morphological complexity, and other factors.Less
This chapter argues that frequency, prosody, and morphology interact in principled ways to yield quantitative asymmetries in the morphosyntactic realization of the English comparative. The typical generalization is that length of the adjective determines whether or not -er is augmented by arguing that usage frequency creates distinct lexical entries for frequent and infrequent adjectives. In accordance with independent facts of differential susceptibility to lenition and phonetic reduction, the chapter argues that the relevant locus of difference lies in prosodic detail. Broadly, the comparative suffix -er is more amenable to attachment to prosodically incomplete adjectives, given its status as a cohering suffix. Under the theoretical principle of comparative grammaticality, whereby a competing way of expressing semantic comparison is available, the degree to which -er attaches to adjectives varies as a function of the prosodic completeness of the adjective, its morphological complexity, and other factors.
Peter Barber
- Published in print:
- 2013
- Published Online:
- April 2014
- ISBN:
- 9780199680504
- eISBN:
- 9780191760525
- Item type:
- book
- Publisher:
- Oxford University Press
- DOI:
- 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199680504.001.0001
- Subject:
- Classical Studies, Literary Studies: Classical, Early, and Medieval
This book is an investigation of how semivowels were realised in Indo-European and in early Greek. More specifically, it examines the extent to which Indo-European *i and *y were independent ...
More
This book is an investigation of how semivowels were realised in Indo-European and in early Greek. More specifically, it examines the extent to which Indo-European *i and *y were independent phonemes, in what respects their alternation was predictable, and how this situation changed as Indo-European developed into Greek. Evidence from Greek, Germanic and Vedic are crucial for understanding the Indo-European situation; this book undertakes a re-examination of the evidence provided by Gothic and Vedic, and offers the first comprehensive survey of the Greek evidence. The impact of this evidence on the theories of Sievers, Edgerton, Lindeman, Schindler and Seebold is assessed. This inquiry has significant morphological as well as phonological components; a proper understanding of the early behaviour of semivowels depends on disentangling considerable morphological innovation in the comparative adjectives in *-yos-/-iyos-, the nominals in *-ye/o-, *-iye/o-, *-y-e/o-, *-i-(y)e/o-, and *-tye/o-, the feminine suffix *-ya, and verbal formations in *-ye/o- (and to a limited extent *-i-(y)e/o). The evidence provided by optatives in *-yeH 1- and morphological categories showing the effects of assibilation is also assessed. The comprehensive nature of this study, its sensitivity to questions of relative chronology, and careful assessment of what is inherited and what is innovative, enable substantive conclusions to be drawn regarding the behaviour of semivowels at various stages in the history of Greek and in Indo-European itself. In turn these conclusions bear on such questions as the interaction of semivowel syllabicity with syllable and foot structure, sandhi phenomena, and the moraic properties of obstruents (including laryngeals).Less
This book is an investigation of how semivowels were realised in Indo-European and in early Greek. More specifically, it examines the extent to which Indo-European *i and *y were independent phonemes, in what respects their alternation was predictable, and how this situation changed as Indo-European developed into Greek. Evidence from Greek, Germanic and Vedic are crucial for understanding the Indo-European situation; this book undertakes a re-examination of the evidence provided by Gothic and Vedic, and offers the first comprehensive survey of the Greek evidence. The impact of this evidence on the theories of Sievers, Edgerton, Lindeman, Schindler and Seebold is assessed. This inquiry has significant morphological as well as phonological components; a proper understanding of the early behaviour of semivowels depends on disentangling considerable morphological innovation in the comparative adjectives in *-yos-/-iyos-, the nominals in *-ye/o-, *-iye/o-, *-y-e/o-, *-i-(y)e/o-, and *-tye/o-, the feminine suffix *-ya, and verbal formations in *-ye/o- (and to a limited extent *-i-(y)e/o). The evidence provided by optatives in *-yeH 1- and morphological categories showing the effects of assibilation is also assessed. The comprehensive nature of this study, its sensitivity to questions of relative chronology, and careful assessment of what is inherited and what is innovative, enable substantive conclusions to be drawn regarding the behaviour of semivowels at various stages in the history of Greek and in Indo-European itself. In turn these conclusions bear on such questions as the interaction of semivowel syllabicity with syllable and foot structure, sandhi phenomena, and the moraic properties of obstruents (including laryngeals).
Herman Cappelen and Ernie Lepore
- Published in print:
- 2015
- Published Online:
- January 2016
- ISBN:
- 9780199641338
- eISBN:
- 9780191813498
- Item type:
- chapter
- Publisher:
- Oxford University Press
- DOI:
- 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199641338.003.0007
- Subject:
- Philosophy, Aesthetics
This chapter sets out to defend semantic minimalism. Semantic minimalism holds inter alia that there are relatively few context-sensitive expressions (essentially just the obvious cases involving ...
More
This chapter sets out to defend semantic minimalism. Semantic minimalism holds inter alia that there are relatively few context-sensitive expressions (essentially just the obvious cases involving indexicals, demonstratives, etc.) and that the context of utterance has little to no effect on semantic propositions or truth conditions. Against this thesis, opponents have launched two common objections, (1) it entails that comparative adjectives are context insensitive and (2) it renders the role of semantics in communication inexplicable. In response, it is argued that (1) can be accepted and explained through Speech Act Pluralism while (2) is more problematic for the opponents of semantic minimalism as their accounts of communicated content fail certain tests of psychological reality.Less
This chapter sets out to defend semantic minimalism. Semantic minimalism holds inter alia that there are relatively few context-sensitive expressions (essentially just the obvious cases involving indexicals, demonstratives, etc.) and that the context of utterance has little to no effect on semantic propositions or truth conditions. Against this thesis, opponents have launched two common objections, (1) it entails that comparative adjectives are context insensitive and (2) it renders the role of semantics in communication inexplicable. In response, it is argued that (1) can be accepted and explained through Speech Act Pluralism while (2) is more problematic for the opponents of semantic minimalism as their accounts of communicated content fail certain tests of psychological reality.
Rebecca Treiman
- Published in print:
- 1993
- Published Online:
- November 2020
- ISBN:
- 9780195062199
- eISBN:
- 9780197560143
- Item type:
- chapter
- Publisher:
- Oxford University Press
- DOI:
- 10.1093/oso/9780195062199.003.0013
- Subject:
- Education, Early Childhood and Elementary Education
In this chapter, I discuss the first graders’ spellings of inflected and derived words. The children in this study often misspelled inflected words (Chapter 2). One type of error that has already ...
More
In this chapter, I discuss the first graders’ spellings of inflected and derived words. The children in this study often misspelled inflected words (Chapter 2). One type of error that has already been documented is the omission of inflectional endings like the /s/ of books (Chapter 8). This chapter considers the children’s spellings of inflected and derived words in more detail. Before beginning the discussion, some definitions and examples are in order. In English, inflections are added to the ends of words to mark such things as tense and number. For example, helped contains the verb stem help plus the past tense inflectional suffix. I refer to the past tense suffix as -D. Helped contains two morphemes or units of meaning, help and -D. The inflected word books also contains two morphemes, the stem book and the plural suffix -Z. As these examples show, the addition of an inflectional suffix does not change a word’s part of speech. Derivations differ in several ways from inflections. For one thing, English derivational morphemes may be either prefixes or suffixes. One derivational prefix is re-, which may be added to the verb read to form reread. Derivational suffixes include -ion and -ly. Unlike inflections, derivations may change a word’s part of speech. For example, the noun vacation is derived from the verb vacate by the addition of-ion; the adjective facial is derived from the noun face by the addition of -ial. The relation in meaning between a stem and a derived form is often less transparent than the relation in meaning between a stem and an inflected form. For instance, one cannot predict the full meaning of vacation from the meaning of its parts. As discussed in Chapter 1, the spellings of inflected and derived words in English often represent the words’ morphemic forms rather than their phonemic forms. For example, the past tense suffix is /t/ in words like helped, whose stem ends with a voiceless consonant, but /d/ in words like cleaned, whose stem ends with a voiced consonant. The phonemic forms of stems, too, sometimes change when inflectional or derivational morphemes are added.
Less
In this chapter, I discuss the first graders’ spellings of inflected and derived words. The children in this study often misspelled inflected words (Chapter 2). One type of error that has already been documented is the omission of inflectional endings like the /s/ of books (Chapter 8). This chapter considers the children’s spellings of inflected and derived words in more detail. Before beginning the discussion, some definitions and examples are in order. In English, inflections are added to the ends of words to mark such things as tense and number. For example, helped contains the verb stem help plus the past tense inflectional suffix. I refer to the past tense suffix as -D. Helped contains two morphemes or units of meaning, help and -D. The inflected word books also contains two morphemes, the stem book and the plural suffix -Z. As these examples show, the addition of an inflectional suffix does not change a word’s part of speech. Derivations differ in several ways from inflections. For one thing, English derivational morphemes may be either prefixes or suffixes. One derivational prefix is re-, which may be added to the verb read to form reread. Derivational suffixes include -ion and -ly. Unlike inflections, derivations may change a word’s part of speech. For example, the noun vacation is derived from the verb vacate by the addition of-ion; the adjective facial is derived from the noun face by the addition of -ial. The relation in meaning between a stem and a derived form is often less transparent than the relation in meaning between a stem and an inflected form. For instance, one cannot predict the full meaning of vacation from the meaning of its parts. As discussed in Chapter 1, the spellings of inflected and derived words in English often represent the words’ morphemic forms rather than their phonemic forms. For example, the past tense suffix is /t/ in words like helped, whose stem ends with a voiceless consonant, but /d/ in words like cleaned, whose stem ends with a voiced consonant. The phonemic forms of stems, too, sometimes change when inflectional or derivational morphemes are added.