Marcus Hartner
- Published in print:
- 2017
- Published Online:
- January 2017
- ISBN:
- 9780190496869
- eISBN:
- 9780190496883
- Item type:
- chapter
- Publisher:
- Oxford University Press
- DOI:
- 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780190496869.003.0002
- Subject:
- Linguistics, Psycholinguistics / Neurolinguistics / Cognitive Linguistics
The past decade has witnessed a growing interest in cognitive approaches within the humanities. Yet particularly in the study of literature, many scholars remain sceptical towards this development. ...
More
The past decade has witnessed a growing interest in cognitive approaches within the humanities. Yet particularly in the study of literature, many scholars remain sceptical towards this development. Reasons for this may lie in the field’s lack of a common theoretical framework, or an old-fashioned concern about the ‘infiltration and contamination [of literary criticism] by other disciplines’ (Waugh). Another reason can be found in the epistemological and methodological uncertainties surrounding the intersection of empirical (cognitive) science and (non-empirical) literary studies. Problems such as the ‘explanatory gap’ between mind and brain, or the different aims of scientific and literary research have not escaped literary criticism. They have given rise to a critical debate on the potential, the scope, and the problems of cognitive approaches. This chapter engages with this discussion about general conceptual criteria for the academic meeting of literature and science from the perspective of literary scholarship. Drawing on Patrick Hogan’s model of the structural relationship between different levels of scientific investigation, and the work of critics such as Marie-Laure Ryan, William Paulson, and Patricia Waugh, it aims to outline a set of theoretical and methodological criteria for cognitive literary studies. Specifically, the chapter develops and proposes three broad guiding principles (coherence, moderation, and autonomy) for the design of cognitive approaches to literature. By elaborating on the conceptual underpinnings and the practical consequences of those guidelines, it provides some conceptual orientation for research situated at the intersection of literature and science. In this way, it also aims to undercut some of the methodological criticism directed at this exciting interdisciplinary field of study.Less
The past decade has witnessed a growing interest in cognitive approaches within the humanities. Yet particularly in the study of literature, many scholars remain sceptical towards this development. Reasons for this may lie in the field’s lack of a common theoretical framework, or an old-fashioned concern about the ‘infiltration and contamination [of literary criticism] by other disciplines’ (Waugh). Another reason can be found in the epistemological and methodological uncertainties surrounding the intersection of empirical (cognitive) science and (non-empirical) literary studies. Problems such as the ‘explanatory gap’ between mind and brain, or the different aims of scientific and literary research have not escaped literary criticism. They have given rise to a critical debate on the potential, the scope, and the problems of cognitive approaches. This chapter engages with this discussion about general conceptual criteria for the academic meeting of literature and science from the perspective of literary scholarship. Drawing on Patrick Hogan’s model of the structural relationship between different levels of scientific investigation, and the work of critics such as Marie-Laure Ryan, William Paulson, and Patricia Waugh, it aims to outline a set of theoretical and methodological criteria for cognitive literary studies. Specifically, the chapter develops and proposes three broad guiding principles (coherence, moderation, and autonomy) for the design of cognitive approaches to literature. By elaborating on the conceptual underpinnings and the practical consequences of those guidelines, it provides some conceptual orientation for research situated at the intersection of literature and science. In this way, it also aims to undercut some of the methodological criticism directed at this exciting interdisciplinary field of study.
Daniel M. Gross
- Published in print:
- 2017
- Published Online:
- May 2018
- ISBN:
- 9780226485034
- eISBN:
- 9780226485171
- Item type:
- chapter
- Publisher:
- University of Chicago Press
- DOI:
- 10.7208/chicago/9780226485171.003.0002
- Subject:
- Literature, Criticism/Theory
Chapter 1 argues that Charles Darwin's The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals can serve as a foundational text for a humanities approach to emotion, but only if it is first disentangled ...
More
Chapter 1 argues that Charles Darwin's The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals can serve as a foundational text for a humanities approach to emotion, but only if it is first disentangled from its heavy-handed editor Paul Ekman and his basic emotions program, which has done Darwin a profound disservice. So liberated, the chapter then argues, Darwin's science of emotion provides a reference point for scholars in the humanities now trying to make their literary criticism speak to natural science. The photograph, illustration, and story-filled Expression is both rhetorical and inseparable from its science that we sometimes imagine transcending its bookish material. This chapter recalls how Darwin's Expression foregrounds the inherent rhetoricity of emotion, thereby outstripping Ekman's science of emotion that claims to follow in its wake, and which has recently found some advocates in the new subfield of Cognitive Approaches to Literature. Instead, the chapter argues that Darwin's rhetoric of emotion is remarkably skeptical, which does not diminish its scientific piquancy, but rather aligns it with our situated theories in the science of cognition mobilized, among other places, by the philosopher of biology Alva Noë.Less
Chapter 1 argues that Charles Darwin's The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals can serve as a foundational text for a humanities approach to emotion, but only if it is first disentangled from its heavy-handed editor Paul Ekman and his basic emotions program, which has done Darwin a profound disservice. So liberated, the chapter then argues, Darwin's science of emotion provides a reference point for scholars in the humanities now trying to make their literary criticism speak to natural science. The photograph, illustration, and story-filled Expression is both rhetorical and inseparable from its science that we sometimes imagine transcending its bookish material. This chapter recalls how Darwin's Expression foregrounds the inherent rhetoricity of emotion, thereby outstripping Ekman's science of emotion that claims to follow in its wake, and which has recently found some advocates in the new subfield of Cognitive Approaches to Literature. Instead, the chapter argues that Darwin's rhetoric of emotion is remarkably skeptical, which does not diminish its scientific piquancy, but rather aligns it with our situated theories in the science of cognition mobilized, among other places, by the philosopher of biology Alva Noë.
Caroline Pirlet and Andreas Wirag
- Published in print:
- 2017
- Published Online:
- January 2017
- ISBN:
- 9780190496869
- eISBN:
- 9780190496883
- Item type:
- chapter
- Publisher:
- Oxford University Press
- DOI:
- 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780190496869.003.0003
- Subject:
- Linguistics, Psycholinguistics / Neurolinguistics / Cognitive Linguistics
The chapter explores the neglected affective dimension in reader-response frameworks in cognitive poetics and narratology to advance a more holistic model of how readers make sense of stories. Future ...
More
The chapter explores the neglected affective dimension in reader-response frameworks in cognitive poetics and narratology to advance a more holistic model of how readers make sense of stories. Future narratological frameworks need to accommodate emotion-derived reader responses in story comprehension as a complement to traditional, more narrowly intellect-based reader cognitions. The argument is divided into three sections. First, reiterating Stockwell’s conviction that affect is integral to understanding fictional narrative, a theory of ‘literary emotions’ is proposed in order to account for affective reader responses to fiction. A distinction is highlighted between discrete ‘emotionologies’ (i.e. culture-specific forms of emotional sense-making that produce idiosyncratic reader responses) and an evolutionary repertoire of basic emotions that warrants intercultural intelligibility of fiction. Following this exposition, a close reading of James Joyce’s short story ‘Eveline’ (1914) illustrates the interplay of affective and cognitive-rational components in decision-making processes that surface in fictional characters and reading audiences alike. The hapless heroine epitomizes the topical struggle between pragmatic and emotional contemplations as she unsuccessfully strives to break free from stifling personal circumstances. Readers, by the same token, will find themselves hesitant to condemn the heroine’s indecision as their empathic responses towards Eveline influence their evaluation of the fictional other. As a contribution to narratological theory formation, finally, an appendix attempts to jointly theorize affective and cognitive-intellectual reader responses within the unified framework of a ‘natural’ narratology. Monika Fludernik’s model, due to its emphasis on readers’ cognitive-constructivist construals of narrative, provides a theoretical model well suited to accommodating affective reader responses to literature.Less
The chapter explores the neglected affective dimension in reader-response frameworks in cognitive poetics and narratology to advance a more holistic model of how readers make sense of stories. Future narratological frameworks need to accommodate emotion-derived reader responses in story comprehension as a complement to traditional, more narrowly intellect-based reader cognitions. The argument is divided into three sections. First, reiterating Stockwell’s conviction that affect is integral to understanding fictional narrative, a theory of ‘literary emotions’ is proposed in order to account for affective reader responses to fiction. A distinction is highlighted between discrete ‘emotionologies’ (i.e. culture-specific forms of emotional sense-making that produce idiosyncratic reader responses) and an evolutionary repertoire of basic emotions that warrants intercultural intelligibility of fiction. Following this exposition, a close reading of James Joyce’s short story ‘Eveline’ (1914) illustrates the interplay of affective and cognitive-rational components in decision-making processes that surface in fictional characters and reading audiences alike. The hapless heroine epitomizes the topical struggle between pragmatic and emotional contemplations as she unsuccessfully strives to break free from stifling personal circumstances. Readers, by the same token, will find themselves hesitant to condemn the heroine’s indecision as their empathic responses towards Eveline influence their evaluation of the fictional other. As a contribution to narratological theory formation, finally, an appendix attempts to jointly theorize affective and cognitive-intellectual reader responses within the unified framework of a ‘natural’ narratology. Monika Fludernik’s model, due to its emphasis on readers’ cognitive-constructivist construals of narrative, provides a theoretical model well suited to accommodating affective reader responses to literature.
Michael Burke and Emily T. Troscianko (eds)
- Published in print:
- 2017
- Published Online:
- January 2017
- ISBN:
- 9780190496869
- eISBN:
- 9780190496883
- Item type:
- book
- Publisher:
- Oxford University Press
- DOI:
- 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780190496869.001.0001
- Subject:
- Linguistics, Psycholinguistics / Neurolinguistics / Cognitive Linguistics
This book brings together researchers with cognitive-scientific and literary backgrounds to present innovative research in all three variations on the possible interactions between literary studies ...
More
This book brings together researchers with cognitive-scientific and literary backgrounds to present innovative research in all three variations on the possible interactions between literary studies and cognitive science. The tripartite structure of the volume reflects a more ambitious conception of what cognitive approaches to literature are and could be than is usually encountered, and thus aims both to map out and to advance the field. The first section corresponds to what most people think of as ‘cognitive poetics’ or ‘cognitive literary studies’: the study of literature by literary scholars drawing on cognitive-scientific methods, findings, and/or debates to yield insights into literature. The second section demonstrates that literary scholars need not only make use of cognitive science to study literature, but can also, in a reciprocally interdisciplinary manner, use a cognitively informed perspective on literature to offer benefits back to the cognitive sciences. Finally, the third section, ‘literature in cognitive science’, showcases some of the ways in which literature can be a stimulating object of study and a fertile testing ground for theories and models, not only to literary scholars but also to cognitive scientists, who here engage with some key questions in cognitive literary studies with the benefit of their in-depth scientific knowledge and training.Less
This book brings together researchers with cognitive-scientific and literary backgrounds to present innovative research in all three variations on the possible interactions between literary studies and cognitive science. The tripartite structure of the volume reflects a more ambitious conception of what cognitive approaches to literature are and could be than is usually encountered, and thus aims both to map out and to advance the field. The first section corresponds to what most people think of as ‘cognitive poetics’ or ‘cognitive literary studies’: the study of literature by literary scholars drawing on cognitive-scientific methods, findings, and/or debates to yield insights into literature. The second section demonstrates that literary scholars need not only make use of cognitive science to study literature, but can also, in a reciprocally interdisciplinary manner, use a cognitively informed perspective on literature to offer benefits back to the cognitive sciences. Finally, the third section, ‘literature in cognitive science’, showcases some of the ways in which literature can be a stimulating object of study and a fertile testing ground for theories and models, not only to literary scholars but also to cognitive scientists, who here engage with some key questions in cognitive literary studies with the benefit of their in-depth scientific knowledge and training.